Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Accepted Manuscripts
    • Article Preview
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Video Articles
    • AJNR Case Collection
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
  • Special Collections
    • AJNR Awards
    • Low-Field MRI
    • Alzheimer Disease
    • ASNR Foundation Special Collection
    • Photon-Counting CT
    • View All
  • Multimedia
    • AJNR Podcasts
    • AJNR SCANtastic
    • Trainee Corner
    • MRI Safety Corner
    • Imaging Protocols
  • For Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Submit a Video Article
    • Submit an eLetter to the Editor/Response
    • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
    • Statistical Tips
    • Fast Publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
    • Graphical Abstract Preparation
    • Imaging Protocol Submission
    • Author Policies
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Board Alumni
  • More
    • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Advertisers
    • ASNR Home

User menu

  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
American Journal of Neuroradiology
American Journal of Neuroradiology

American Journal of Neuroradiology

ASHNR American Society of Functional Neuroradiology ASHNR American Society of Pediatric Neuroradiology ASSR
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Accepted Manuscripts
    • Article Preview
    • Past Issue Archive
    • Video Articles
    • AJNR Case Collection
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
  • Special Collections
    • AJNR Awards
    • Low-Field MRI
    • Alzheimer Disease
    • ASNR Foundation Special Collection
    • Photon-Counting CT
    • View All
  • Multimedia
    • AJNR Podcasts
    • AJNR SCANtastic
    • Trainee Corner
    • MRI Safety Corner
    • Imaging Protocols
  • For Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Submit a Video Article
    • Submit an eLetter to the Editor/Response
    • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
    • Statistical Tips
    • Fast Publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
    • Graphical Abstract Preparation
    • Imaging Protocol Submission
    • Author Policies
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Board Alumni
  • More
    • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Advertisers
    • ASNR Home
  • Follow AJNR on Twitter
  • Visit AJNR on Facebook
  • Follow AJNR on Instagram
  • Join AJNR on LinkedIn
  • RSS Feeds

AJNR Awards, New Junior Editors, and more. Read the latest AJNR updates

Research ArticleFunctional
Open Access

Functional MR Imaging Evidence of Altered Functional Activation in Metabolic Syndrome

K.F. Hoth, M.M. Gonzales, T. Tarumi, S.C. Miles, H. Tanaka and A.P. Haley
American Journal of Neuroradiology March 2011, 32 (3) 541-547; DOI: https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A2315
K.F. Hoth
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
M.M. Gonzales
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
T. Tarumi
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
S.C. Miles
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
H. Tanaka
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
A.P. Haley
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Responses
  • References
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: MetS is a cluster of risk factors associated with significant cardiovascular morbidity and mortality and diminished cognitive function. Given that little is known about the early signs of brain vulnerability related to persistent metabolic dysfunction, we set out to determine whether cognitively healthy middle-aged individuals with MetS exhibit an altered cerebrovascular response to a cognitive challenge relative to those without MetS.

MATERIALS AND METHODS: Forty neurologically healthy adults aged 40–60 years (19 with MetS and 21 healthy controls) performed a 2-back verbal working memory task during fMRI. We compared BOLD responses between the 2 groups in 8 a priori regions of interest previously shown to be associated with the 2-back in patients with cardiovascular disease.

RESULTS: Age, education level, sex distribution, cognitive and emotional functioning, and task performance (accuracy and reaction time) were not different between the groups. Compared with healthy controls, individuals with MetS demonstrated a lower 2-back–related BOLD response in the right superior frontal gyrus, right superior parietal lobule, and left inferior parietal lobule.

CONCLUSIONS: This study provides preliminary evidence that cognitively intact middle-aged individuals with MetS exhibit significant alterations in cerebrovascular response to a cognitive challenge. Our results also demonstrate that fMRI may identify early brain changes associated with MetS.

Abbreviations

AD
Alzheimer disease
AFNI
Analysis of Functional NeuroImages
BMI
body mass index
BOLD
blood oxygen level–dependent
CVLT-II
California Verbal Learning Test-II
EPI
echo-planar imaging
fMRI
functional MR imaging
HDL cholesterol
high-density lipoprotein cholesterol
HIV
human immunodeficiency virus
IQ
intelligence quotient
LDL cholesterol
low-density lipoprotein cholesterol
MetS
metabolic syndrome
MMSE
Mini-Mental Status Examination
MS
multiple sclerosis
RCF
Rey Complex Figure Test
WASI
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
WM
white matter

The number of elderly patients with dementia is expected to quadruple during the next 50 years.1 In addition, a great number of older adults are projected to develop milder forms of cognitive impairment.1 This trend is alarming because cognition is the most important determinant of health status, quality of life, and functional ability in older age.2 Diminished cognitive capacity causes significant psychological, social, and economic hardship and adversely impacts a person's ability to benefit from treatment for other medical problems.3 Due to the current paucity of treatments for dementia, management of treatable factors that place the brain at risk for subsequent cognitive decline is the primary line of defense, especially in cases in which vascular factors play a key role.4–7

MetS refers to a clustering of risk factors (ie, hypertension, hyperglycemia, dyslipidemia, and obesity) associated with increased cardiovascular morbidity and mortality.8 Growing evidence suggests that MetS is also harmful to cognition and that the cluster may have predictive value for cognitive decline over and above that of its individual components.9–16 Older patients with MetS score lower on cognitive tests than age-matched healthy adults, particularly on measures of processing speed and executive functioning,17 cognitive domains that are associated with vascular cognitive impairment. Several studies have shown that middle-aged individuals with MetS are at higher risk of developing dementia in late life,18 yet accurate prediction of individual cognitive trajectories is difficult due to the low sensitivity of paper-and-pencil screening tests to subtle changes in intellectual functioning. Neuroimaging studies have shown promise in identifying individuals at risk for subsequent cognitive decline because these studies revealed that MetS is associated with early alterations in the structural integrity of cerebral white matter.19–21

BOLD fMRI has the potential to contribute further to our understanding of early cerebrovascular changes related to persistent metabolic dysfunction. Alterations in the BOLD response to cognitive challenges have been shown in cognitively asymptomatic patients with MS, HIV infection, and genetic risk for AD.22–27 BOLD fMRI performed during cognitive tasks, therefore, can help elucidate the complex relationship between metabolic and vascular health, cerebrovascular support for cognitive function, and behavioral performance. An accurate model of the relationship between cerebrovascular health and persistent metabolic dysfunction at midlife could prove instrumental in increasing the success of broader efforts to prevent and treat vascular cognitive impairment.

To our knowledge, no studies have examined functional neuroimaging in MetS. Accordingly, the aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that middle-aged adults with MetS would show altered patterns of brain activation (ie, BOLD responses) during a verbal working memory task. The chosen task requires selective attention, executive ability, and psychomotor speed, cognitive abilities typically affected in vascular-related cognitive impairment. As such, they may have particular relevance for MetS.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Right-handed28 participants between 40 and 60 years of age were recruited through flyers and newspaper advertisements. Participants were excluded if they had a history of neurologic disease (ie, large-vessel stroke, seizure disorder, Parkinson disease, clinically significant traumatic brain injury, MS, or brain infection/meningitis), major psychiatric illness (eg, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), substance abuse (diagnosed abuse and/or previous hospitalization for substance abuse), or MR imaging contraindications. Forty individuals participated in the study. The mean age of those participants was 50.0 ± 6.0 years (range, 40–60 years; median age, 50 years). The mean education level was 15.2 ± 3.0 years. The mean full-scale IQ score was 115.0 ± 10.5, indicating high average global cognitive functioning according to published norms.29 Enrollees identified themselves as follows: 43% white, 35% Hispanic, 8% African American, 5% Asian American, and 9% other/did not specify.

Procedures

The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 with approval from the local institutional review board. All volunteers provided written informed consent before enrollment. Participants completed a medical history interview with a research assistant. Medical conditions and treatments were coded as either present or absent according to the participant self-report. Participants underwent a general health assessment, a full neuropsychological evaluation, and brain imaging on separate days, completing the study within 1 month.

General Health Assessment and MetS Classification.

Fasting blood concentrations of glucose, triglycerides, HDL cholesterol, and LDL cholesterol were determined by using the standard enzymatic techniques. Arterial blood pressure was measured by using a standard oscillometric blood pressure monitor after at least 15 minutes of rest (VP-2000; Colin Medical Instruments, San Antonio, Texas). BMI was calculated as body weight in kilograms divided by the square of the height in meters. Prescription medications were coded as present or absent according to the participant self-report and were classified as antihypertensive, lipid-lowering, hypoglycemic, antiplatelet, anti-inflammatory, antidepressant, antihistamine, hormone replacement, biphosphonates, or vitamins.

MetS was defined according to the 2009 criteria issued jointly by the International Diabetes Federation, National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, the American Heart Association, World Health Federation, International Atherosclerosis Society, and International Association for the Study of Obesity.8 Participants were included in the MetS group if they fulfilled at least 3 of the flowing criteria: abdominal obesity (BMI of >30 kg/m2, equivalent to waist circumference ≥94 cm for men and ≥80 cm for women8); elevated triglycerides (≥150 mg/dL) or treatment for elevated triglycerides; reduced HDL cholesterol (<40 mg/dL for men and <50 mg/dL for women) or treatment for reduced HDL cholesterol; elevated blood pressure (systolic blood pressure ≥130 mm Hg and/or diastolic blood pressure ≥85 mm Hg) or antihypertensive drug treatment; elevated fasting glucose (≥100 mg/dL) or treatment with a hypoglycemic agent. Participants were currently being treated in the following manner: 8 with antihypertensive medications, 2 with antiplatelet agents, 2 with lipid-lowering agents, 3 with hypoglycemics, 1 with thyroid replacement therapy, 2 with bisphosphonates, and 3 with antidepressant medication. Further details about the demographic and physiologic characteristics of the sample and comparisons between the control and metabolic syndrome groups can be found in Tables 1 and 2.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1:

Selected demographic and physiologic characteristics of subjects

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2:

Cognitive and emotional functioning

Neuropsychological Evaluation.

All participants completed a 2-hour assessment battery, including standard clinical neuropsychological instruments with established reliability and validity.30 The battery included measures of global cognitive functioning (MMSE31; WASI29), language (WASI Vocabulary subtest; Category Fluency for Animals32), memory (CVLT-II33; RCF30), attention-executive functioning (Digit Vigilance 34; Controlled Oral Word Association Test35; Trail Making Test A and B36), psychomotor speed (Grooved Pegboard, Pegs37), visual-spatial ability (RCF copy; WASI Matrix Reasoning subtest), and emotional functioning (Beck Depression Inventory-II).38 All tests were administered and scored by a trained research assistant by using standard administration and scoring criteria.

Neuroimaging.

Each imaging session included working memory task practice, T1-weighted imaging for anatomic reference, and 2 imaging runs of the working memory task. The 2-back task was presented by using E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania) back-projected onto a screen positioned at the participant's head and viewed through a double-mirror attached to the head coil. Responses were collected by using an MR imaging−compatible response box.

MR Imaging Data Acquisition.

MR imaging data for each participant were acquired in a single session on a 3T Signa Excite MR imaging scanner (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, Wisconsin) equipped with a standard head coil. Structural imaging included a high-resolution spoiled gradient-recalled (256 × 256 matrix, FOV = 24 × 24 cm2, 1-mm section thickness, 0 gap) anatomic scan of the entire brain in the sagittal plane. Functional imaging was performed by using a whole-brain EPI sequence (TR = 3000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 24 × 24 cm2, 64 × 64 matrix, 42 axial sections, 3-mm section thickness, 0.3-mm gap).

Working Memory Paradigm.

Working memory was assessed by using a verbal 2-back task. During this task, a series of individual consonants was presented visually for 500 ms each with a 2500-ms interstimulus interval. Consonants were arranged in random order from a list of all consonants except “L” due to ambiguity in the lowercase form. For each stimulus, participants were asked to determine if the letter on the screen was the same as or different from a previously presented letter. Responses were collected by using a 2-button MR imaging−compatible response box. A 0-back control condition was alternated with the 2-back condition in a block design. Two imaging runs, each consisting of 3 blocks of the 0-back, 3 blocks of the 2-back, and 3 blocks of rest presented in alternating order and lasting approximately 6 minutes, were conducted.

0-Back Control Condition.

This task consisted of 3 blocks of 12 consonants of random case and order, 33% of which were targets. Participants responded yes when the upper or lowercase letter H appeared on the screen or no if another letter appeared.

2-Back Working Memory Condition.

The experimental condition consisted of 3 blocks, each containing 15 consonants of random case and order, 33% of which were targets. A letter was considered a target if it was the same as the letter presented 2 stimuli earlier regardless of the case (eg, s, G, S, d, V, D, v, and so forth). Task performance was assessed by measuring accuracy rates and mean reaction time for all correct trials.

Rest.

The participants were instructed to rest for 30 seconds while focusing their attention on a fixation cross that appeared in the middle of the screen.

fMRI Data Processing.

All EPI images were processed by using AFNI software (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni/).39 Each time-series was spatially registered to the sixth volume of the session to reduce the effects of head movement. This AFNI 3D registration program also yielded information on displacement and rotation for each volume, which was used later to further correct motion. All participants moved <1.5 mm per imaging run, and no participants were excluded from the analyses due to excessive head motion. Data preprocessing also included adjustment for differences in adjacent-section timing due to interleaved section acquisition, temporal smoothing, and spatial filtering. Task-related brain activation was determined by using within-subject voxelwise multiple regression analyses with the following parameters: a 0-back/2-back reference waveform convolved with a γ function and covariates accounting for instruction screens and head movement. Averaged task-related activation within a set of 8 a priori regions of interest was examined in subsequent analyses.

A separate dataset was used to create empirically defined task-related regions of interest for hypothesis testing to avoid circularity. The task used to create the regions of interest was identical to the one used in the current study. The sample and the creation of the regions of interest are described in detail in Haley et al.40 Briefly, the regions of interest were created by transforming the results from individual multiple regression analyses to standard stereotaxic space41 and converting them to z scores. The z scores were then thresholded at P < .05, corrected for multiple comparisons by using the false discovery rate supplied by AFNI. Voxels were included in the final mask if they were significantly active in >90% of all participants. Finally, active voxels were defined as a cluster if they were contiguous and formed a volume of at least 200 μL. Eight cortical regions of interest were identified by using this process (Fig 1 and Table 3).

Fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 1.

Regions of interest on template anatomy: 1 = left middle frontal gyrus, 2 = left medial frontal/superior frontal gyrus, 3 = right superior parietal lobule, 4 = left inferior parietal lobule, 5 = left middle frontal gyrus, 6 = right superior frontal gyrus, 7 = right middle frontal gyrus, 8 = right inferior frontal gyrus.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3:

Comparison of averaged task-related brain activation between adults with MetS and controls in a priori regions of interest (MetS < controls)

Anatomic designations were assigned to the regions of interest according to the region-of-interest center in Talairach coordinates. This empirically defined mask was then applied to the results from the individual multiple regression analyses in the current study sample transformed to standard stereotaxic space, by using the fully automated 3dmaskave plug-in in AFNI. This program allows one to compute the average over a region of interest of all voxel values from an input dataset. Average unthresholded t values within each region of interest for each person were used as measures of task-related activation intensity in subsequent analyses for hypothesis testing. These 8 a priori regions of interest were used because they represented the most stable activation pattern in response to the task used in this study from a similar yet separate sample of participants.

Data Analyses

Neuropsychological measures were grouped into 1 of 5 cognitive domains: 1) global cognitive functioning, 2) language functions, 3) visual-spatial abilities, 4) memory functions, and 5) attention-executive-psychomotor functions. The following test scores were included in each domain, and raw total scores were used unless otherwise stated: 1) global: MMSE and WASI full-scale IQ; 2) language: WASI Vocabulary subtest and Category Fluency for Animals; 3) visual-spatial: RCF copy and WASI Matrix Reasoning subtest; 4) memory: CVLT-II immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition discrimination; RCF immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition discrimination; 5) attention-executive-psychomotor functions: Trail Making A and B time to completion, Controlled Oral Word Association Test, WAIS-III Digit Span subtest, and Grooved Pegboard-Dominant Hand time to completion. Participant raw test scores were converted to z scores by using the study sample mean and SD. Timed test scores were multiplied by −1 so that higher scores indicated better performance. Five composite cognitive domain z scores were calculated for each participant by averaging the z scores of all tests within that domain.

All variable distributions were examined by using the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality recommended for small samples. Demographic and physiologic differences between the metabolic syndrome and control groups were assessed by using nonparametric Mann-Whitney U tests due to the fact that many physiologic variables have naturally skewed distributions. Group differences in functional brain activation within the 8 regions of interest were assessed by using analysis of variance. All statistical analyses were performed by using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, Version 16.0 (SPSS, Chicago, Illinois). An α level of .05 was set as the criterion for statistical significance.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Means and SDs of the demographic and physiologic variables for each group are reported in Table 1. Descriptive statistical analyses revealed an ethnically diverse middle-aged sample, well representative of the population of Texas where the study was conducted based on 2000 census data for the state. Nineteen participants fulfilled criteria for inclusion in the MetS group (48%). The remaining 21 participants (52%) were classified as controls. Sex distributions in the 2 groups were comparable (χ2 = 2.63, df = 1, P = .105). As expected, the 2 groups exhibited significantly different systolic and diastolic blood pressures, fasting glucose, BMI, triglyceride, and HDL cholesterol values (Table 1). However, there were no significant group differences in age or education (Table 1). There were also no significant differences in global cognitive function, memory, language, visuospatial ability, attention-executive-psychomotor performance, reported depressive symptoms, or 2-back performance between the adults with MetS and controls (Table 2).

Functional Brain Activation and MetS

2-Back-related activation intensities in the left middle frontal gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus, and right middle frontal gyrus were logarithmically transformed before inclusion in the parametric analysis of variance due to a significant skew in the distributions (Shapiro-Wilk > 0.937, P < .05). Analysis of variance revealed that MetS was associated with significantly lower task-related activation intensity in the right superior frontal gyrus (F(1,38) = 6.91, P = .012), right superior parietal lobule (F(1,38) = 7.65, P = .009), and left inferior parietal lobule (F(1,38) = 7.56, P = .009) (Table 3 and Fig 2). These relationships remained unchanged even after additional adjustment for any potential effects of antihypertensive medication: F(2,37) = 4.68, P = .015, F(2,37) = 5.35, P = .009, F(2,37) = 3.96, P = .028; lipid-lowering medication: F(2,37) = 3.40, P = .044, F(2,37) = 3.80, P = .032, F(2,37) = 4.17, P = .023; or hypoglycemic agents: F(2,37) = 3.37, P = .045, F(2,37) = 4.00, P = .027, F(2,37) = 4.16 P = .023.

Fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 2.

Reduced right superior frontal (A), right superior parietal (B), and left inferior parietal (C) working memory activation in middle-aged adults with MetS compared with age-matched controls. Bars and whiskers represent means and standard errors of the mean. Asterisks indicate significance at P < .05; double asterisks indicate significance at P < .01.

Lower functional activation in the right superior parietal and left inferior parietal lobules was significantly related to longer/slower reaction times (r = −0.35, P = .035 and r = −0.34, P = .037, respectively). In addition, higher functional activation in the left inferior parietal lobule was significantly related to better task accuracy (r = 0.36, P = .03). Trends in a similar direction were found between higher functional activation and better task performance in the right superior parietal lobule (r = 0.30, P = .08) and the right superior frontal gyrus (r = 0.32, P = .05).

Discussion

The salient finding of the present study is that compared with age- and performance-matched healthy controls, middle-aged individuals with MetS demonstrated significantly lower BOLD response to a working memory task on fMRI. A working memory task was specifically chosen because it engages cognitive processes, such as executive function and attention, known to be especially susceptible to vascular cognitive impairment for which MetS is an important risk factor.17 We found that diagnosis of MetS was associated with significantly lower task-related BOLD response in the right superior frontal gyrus, right superior parietal lobule, and left inferior parietal lobule despite intact global cognitive performance. One possible interpretation of these early alterations in the cerebrovascular response to cognition in MetS is that they represent early vulnerability of the frontoparietal executive system to metabolic disturbances. The system is involved in regulating attention, switching attentional focus, and task preparation42 and is known to be affected early in the development of vascular cognitive impairment.43–45 This interpretation is supported by our present finding that greater task-related BOLD response in these regions was associated with better task performance and faster reaction times. The cross-sectional nature of our study limits our ability to infer future cognitive trajectories from the observed functional activation changes. Longitudinal studies that begin in midlife could help validate the use of BOLD fMRI markers as indicators of long-term cognitive outcomes.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine functional imaging differences during a cognitive challenge in MetS, extending the currently available literature documenting structural changes on neuroimaging19–21 and cognitive vulnerability in old age.9–16 A particularly notable aspect of the present study is that MetS-related differences in BOLD response to a demanding cognitive challenge were detected in participants without clinically significant cognitive dysfunction. As evidenced by the widely used exercise stress test, dysfunction in physiologic systems is more likely to manifest when the system is stressed. Early detection of the effects of MetS in middle age by using a cognitive challenge as a stressor has the potential to identify those who may benefit from interventions to halt or delay potentially deleterious effects on the brain. The early cerebrovascular changes we identified highlight the fact that MetS has an impact on cerebral health well before behavioral performance is impaired. These results suggest that developing early interventions for MetS may be important. Diet and exercise have been shown to be effective techniques for attenuating the physiologic disturbances associated with MetS. Our results raise the possibility that early application of risk-factor-reduction strategies may be useful not only for improving physiologic health but also for decreasing the risk of cognitive dysfunction across the lifespan.

fMRI has long been recognized as a promising tool for detecting changes in brain function before individuals meet clinical criteria for cognitive impairment.46 The participants in our MetS group were at increased risk for coronary heart disease and cerebrovascular disease but did not yet have a diagnosis other than MetS. The fact that we observed lower rather than higher BOLD response in this group is also important to consider. In the past, a higher task-related BOLD response at comparable levels of performance in patients with MS, HIV, and genetic risk for AD relative to healthy controls has been observed and interpreted as compensatory overactivation25,46,47; however, the opposite trend (ie, lower task-related activation at similar levels of performance) has been noted in patients at high risk for cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease.42,48–50 On the basis these observations, it may be that early in the vascular disease process, subclinical reductions in vascular responsivity and endothelial function result in lower, yet sufficient, amounts of oxygen being delivered to activated neurons, thus producing a lower BOLD response to a cognitive challenge despite intact behavioral performance.48,49 As vascular problems worsen, further declines in cerebrovascular reactivity and microvascular damage likely lead to vascular cognitive impairment. Support is provided by a growing body of literature documenting that peripheral cardiovascular dysfunction is related to poor cerebrovascular health and diminished cognitive function in older patients with cardiovascular disease.43–45,51–55 Due to the multifaceted nature of MetS, further research is necessary to explore the contributions of frank vascular disease to the observed cognitive task−related BOLD response changes in midlife. Further exploration of other potential mechanisms such as alterations in cerebral glucose metabolism and inflammation will also be valuable.

While considering future directions, it is important to discuss the strengths and limitations of the present study. An important strength of the study is the thorough characterization of our sample in terms of cognitive and physiologic functioning. The study used a full cognitive battery as well as measures of metabolic and blood pressure function rather than relying on self-reported medical history. The confirmed cognitive and physiologic health status allows us to interpret the detected functional activation changes with greater confidence, without suspecting confounding effects of under-reported or undiagnosed medical conditions or cognitive disorders. On the other hand, the study was limited to a relatively small sample of self-selected community volunteers. Ideally, the external validity of the results should be tested in larger randomly selected community samples. A larger sample would also allow the exploration of the complex interactions between the various components of metabolic syndrome. In addition, concomitant assessments of white matter integrity may be useful in future studies to further elucidate the relationships detected in this study. While overt signs of microvascular damage in early adulthood are rare, small white matter lesions have been reported in randomly selected individuals in their 40s and 50s.56 Therefore, assessments of white matter integrity may lend further insights into cerebral metabolic health in middle-aged individuals. Additional measures of BOLD response to noncognitive tasks and hypercapnia, on the other hand, can help determine whether the MetS-related alterations in the cerebrovascular support for cognition reported in this study are specific to brain areas critical for cognitive function and/or related to global changes in cerebral perfusion, cerebrovascular reactivity, or cerebrovascular coupling. Finally, it is important to validate the idea that early alterations in cognitive task-related BOLD response may be indicators of a future cognitive trajectory, which should be assessed in longitudinal studies that begin in midlife and track long-term cognitive outcomes.

Conclusions

In summary, we present the first study to examine fMRI in MetS. Our preliminary results indicate that middle-aged individuals with MetS demonstrate significantly lower BOLD response to a working memory challenge. Thus, fMRI may be a helpful tool to identify early cerebrovascular changes associated with MetS. Future studies including BOLD response to cognitive and noncognitive challenges as well as measures of cerebral perfusion and white matter integrity will be important. These approaches will elucidate the physiologic mechanisms underlying the observed alterations in BOLD response to working memory among middle-aged individuals with MetS. Larger sample sizes will allow examination of the individual MetS components as well as the cumulative effect of the constellation of symptoms.

Acknowledgments

We thank the University of Texas Imaging Research Center Staff for their help with the participants.

Footnotes

  • This work was funded by American Heart Association grant 09BGIA2060722 (A.P.H.) and the University of Texas at Austin (A.P.H.).

Indicates open access to non-subscribers at www.ajnr.org

References

  1. 1.↵
    The Silver Book: Chronic Disease and Medical Innovation in an Aging Nation. http://www.silverbook.org. Accessed 2007
  2. 2.↵
    1. Gaugler JE,
    2. Yu F,
    3. Krichbaum K,
    4. et al
    . Predictors of nursing home admission for persons with dementia. Med Care 2009;47:191–98
    CrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    1. Athilingam P,
    2. King KB
    . Heart and brain matters in heart failure: a literature review. J N Y State Nurses Assoc 2007;38:13–19
    PubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Erkinjuntti T,
    2. Roman G,
    3. Gauthier S,
    4. et al
    . Emerging therapies for vascular dementia and vascular cognitive impairment. Stroke 2004;35:1010–17
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  5. 5.↵
    1. Erkinjuntti T
    . Diagnosis and management of vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. J Neural Transm Suppl 2002;91–109
  6. 6.↵
    1. Farlow MR
    . Use of antidementia agents in vascular dementia: beyond Alzheimer disease. Mayo Clin Proc 2006;81:1350–58
    CrossRefPubMed
  7. 7.↵
    1. Nyenhuis DL,
    2. Gorelick PB
    . Diagnosis and management of vascular cognitive impairment. Curr Atheroscler Rep 2007;9:326–32
    CrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Alberti KG,
    2. Eckel RH,
    3. Grundy SM,
    4. et al
    . Harmonizing the metabolic syndrome: a joint interim statement of the International Diabetes Federation Task Force on Epidemiology and Prevention; National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute; American Heart Association; World Heart Federation; International Atherosclerosis Society; and International Association for the Study of Obesity. Circulation 2009;120:1640–45. Epub 2009 Oct 5
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  9. 9.↵
    1. Akbaraly TN,
    2. Kivimaki M,
    3. Shipley MJ,
    4. et al
    . Metabolic syndrome over 10 years and cognitive functioning in late midlife: the Whitehall II study. Diabetes Care 2010;33:84–89. Epub 2009 Oct 16
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  10. 10.↵
    1. Kalmijn S,
    2. Foley D,
    3. White L,
    4. et al
    . Metabolic cardiovascular syndrome and risk of dementia in Japanese-American elderly men: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 2000;20:2255–60
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  11. 11.↵
    1. Yaffe K,
    2. Haan M,
    3. Blackwell T,
    4. et al
    . Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline in elderly Latinos: findings from the Sacramento Area Latino Study of Aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007;55:758–62
    CrossRefPubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Yaffe K,
    2. Weston AL,
    3. Blackwell T,
    4. et al
    . The metabolic syndrome and development of cognitive impairment among older women. Arch Neurol 2009;66:324–28
    CrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    1. Gatto NM,
    2. Henderson VW,
    3. St John JA,
    4. et al
    . Metabolic syndrome and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged and older adults without diabetes. Neuropsychol Dev Cog B Neuropsychol Cog 2008;15:627–41. Epub 2008 May 16
  14. 14.↵
    1. Dik MG,
    2. Jonker C,
    3. Comijs HC,
    4. et al
    . Contribution of metabolic syndrome components to cognition in older individuals. Diabetes Care 2007;30:2655–60
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  15. 15.↵
    1. Vanhanen M,
    2. Koivisto K,
    3. Moilanen L,
    4. et al
    . Association of metabolic syndrome with Alzheimer disease: a population-based study. Neurology 2006;67:843–47
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  16. 16.↵
    1. Raffaitin C,
    2. Gin H,
    3. Empana J-P,
    4. et al
    . Metabolic syndrome and risk for incident Alzheimer's disease or vascular dementia: the Three-City Study. Diabetes Care 2009;32:169–74
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  17. 17.↵
    1. Segura B,
    2. Jurado MA,
    3. Freixenet N,
    4. et al
    . Mental slowness and executive dysfunctions in patients with metabolic syndrome. Neurosci Lett 2009;462:49–53
    CrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Yaffe K
    . Metabolic syndrome and cognitive disorders: is the sum greater than its parts? Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2007;21:167–71
    CrossRefPubMed
  19. 19.↵
    1. Bokura H,
    2. Yamaguchi S,
    3. Iijima K,
    4. et al
    . Metabolic syndrome is associated with silent ischemic brain lesions. Stroke 2008;39:1607–09
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  20. 20.↵
    1. Segura B,
    2. Jurado MA,
    3. Freixenet N,
    4. et al
    . Microstructural white matter changes in metabolic syndrome: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurology 2009;73:438–44
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  21. 21.↵
    1. Park K,
    2. Yasuda N,
    3. Toyonaga S,
    4. et al
    . Significant associations of metabolic syndrome and its components with silent lacunar infarction in middle aged subjects. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:719–21
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  22. 22.↵
    1. Sweet LH,
    2. Rao SM,
    3. Primeau M,
    4. et al
    . Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory among multiple sclerosis patients. J Neuroimaging 2004;14:150–57
    PubMed
  23. 23.↵
    1. Machulda MM,
    2. Ward HA,
    3. Borowski B,
    4. et al
    . Comparison of memory fMRI response among normal, MCI, and Alzheimer's patients. Neurology 2003;61:500–06
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  24. 24.↵
    1. Saykin AJ,
    2. Flashman LA,
    3. Frutiger SA,
    4. et al
    . Neuroanatomic substrates of semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: patterns of functional MRI activation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1999;5:377–92
    CrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Chang L,
    2. Speck O,
    3. Miller EN,
    4. et al
    . Neural correlates of attention and working memory deficits in HIV patients. Neurology 2001;57:1001–07
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  26. 26.↵
    1. Bondi MW,
    2. Houston WS,
    3. Eyler LT,
    4. et al
    . fMRI evidence of compensatory mechanisms in older adults at genetic risk for Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2005;64:501–08
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  27. 27.↵
    1. Filbey FM,
    2. Slack KJ,
    3. Sunderland TP,
    4. et al
    . Functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetoencephalography differences associated with APOE ε4 in young healthy adults. Neuroreport 2006;17:1585–90
    CrossRefPubMed
  28. 28.↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    . The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97–113
    CrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Wechsler D
    . Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Manual. San Antonio, Texas: Harcourt Assessment Company; 1999
  30. 30.↵
    1. Lezak MD
    . Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995
  31. 31.↵
    1. Folstein MF,
    2. Folstein SE,
    3. McHugh PR
    . “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98
    CrossRefPubMed
  32. 32.↵
    1. Morris JC,
    2. Heyman A,
    3. Mohs RC,
    4. et al
    . The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1989;39:1159–65
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  33. 33.↵
    1. Delis DC,
    2. Kramer JH,
    3. Kaplan E,
    4. et al
    . California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) Manual. San Antonio, Texas: Harcourt Assessment Company; 2000
  34. 34.↵
    1. Lewis RF
    . Digit Vigilance Test. Odessa, California: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995
  35. 35.↵
    1. Eslinger P
    . The Iowa Screening Battery for Mental Decline. Iowa City, Iowa: University of Iowa College of Medicine; 1984
  36. 36.↵
    1. Reitan R
    . Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958;8:271–76
    CrossRef
  37. 37.↵
    1. Klove H,
    2. Forster FM
    . Clinical neuropsychology. In: The Medical Clinics of North America. New York: Saunders; 1963
  38. 38.↵
    1. Beck AT,
    2. Steer RA,
    3. Brown GK
    . BDI-II Manual. Fort Worth, Texas: The Psychological Corporation; 1996
  39. 39.↵
    1. Cox RW
    . AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 1996;29:162–73
    CrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    1. Haley AP,
    2. Sweet LH,
    3. Gunstad J,
    4. et al
    . Verbal working memory and atherosclerosis in patients with cardiovascular disease: an fMRI study. J Neuroimaging 2007;17:227–33
    CrossRefPubMed
  41. 41.↵
    1. Talairach J,
    2. Tournoux P
    . Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain 3-D Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers; 1988
  42. 42.↵
    1. Ravizza SM,
    2. Delgado MR,
    3. Chein JM,
    4. et al
    . Functional dissociations within the inferior parietal cortex in verbal working memory. Neuroimage 2004;22:562–73
    CrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    1. Haley AP,
    2. Forman DE,
    3. Poppas A,
    4. et al
    . Carotid artery intima-media thickness and cognition in cardiovascular disease. Int J Cardiol 2007;121:148–54
    CrossRefPubMed
  44. 44.↵
    1. Cohen RA,
    2. Poppas A,
    3. Forman DE,
    4. et al
    . Vascular and cognitive functions associated with cardiovascular disease in the elderly. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2009;31:96–110
    PubMed
  45. 45.↵
    1. Forman DE,
    2. Cohen RA,
    3. Hoth KF,
    4. et al
    . Cognition and brachial endothelial responses in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Artery Research 2008;2:35–43. Epub 2008 Jun 16
    CrossRefPubMed
  46. 46.↵
    1. Brookheimer SY,
    2. Stojwas MH,
    3. Cohen MS,
    4. et al
    . Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med 2000;343:450–56
    CrossRefPubMed
  47. 47.↵
    1. Sweet LH,
    2. Rao SM,
    3. Primeau M,
    4. et al
    . Functional magnetic resonance imaging response to increased verbal working memory demands among patients with multiple sclerosis. Hum Brain Mapp 2006;27:28–36
    CrossRefPubMed
  48. 48.↵
    1. Gonzales MM,
    2. Tarumi T,
    3. Tanaka H,
    4. et al
    . Functional imaging of working memory and peripheral endothelial function in middle-aged adults. Brain Cogn 2010;73:146–51
    CrossRefPubMed
  49. 49.↵
    1. Haley AP,
    2. Gunstad J,
    3. Cohen RA,
    4. et al
    . Neural correlates of visuospatial working memory in healthy young adults at risk for hypertension. Brain Imaging Behav 2008;2:192–99
    CrossRef
  50. 50.↵
    1. Irani F,
    2. Sweet L,
    3. Haley A,
    4. et al
    . An fMRI study of verbal working memory, cardiac output, and ejection fraction in elderly patients with cardiovascular disease. Brain Imaging Behav 2009;3:350–57
    CrossRef
  51. 51.↵
    1. Gunstad J,
    2. Cohen RA,
    3. Tate DF,
    4. et al
    . Blood pressure variability and white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Blood Press 2005;14:353–58
    CrossRefPubMed
  52. 52.↵
    1. Hoth KF,
    2. Tate DF,
    3. Poppas A,
    4. et al
    . Endothelial function and white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease. Stroke 2007;38:308–12. Epub 2007 Jan 4
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  53. 53.↵
    1. Jefferson AL,
    2. Tate DF,
    3. Poppas A,
    4. et al
    . Lower cardiac output is associated with greater white matter hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007;55:1044–48
    CrossRefPubMed
  54. 54.↵
    1. Jefferson AL,
    2. Poppas A,
    3. Paul RH,
    4. et al
    . Systemic hypoperfusion is associated with executive dysfunction in geriatric cardiac patients. Neurobiol Aging 2007;28:477–83
    CrossRefPubMed
  55. 55.↵
    1. Paul RH,
    2. Gunstad J,
    3. Poppas A,
    4. et al
    . Neuroimaging and cardiac correlates of cognitive function among patients with cardiac disease. Cerebrovasc Dis 2005;20:129–33
    PubMed
  56. 56.↵
    1. Sachdev P,
    2. Chen X,
    3. Wen W
    . White matter hyperintensities in mid-adult life. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2008;21:268–74
    CrossRefPubMed
  • Received June 23, 2010.
  • Accepted after revision August 11, 2010.
  • Copyright © American Society of Neuroradiology
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

American Journal of Neuroradiology: 32 (3)
American Journal of Neuroradiology
Vol. 32, Issue 3
1 Mar 2011
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Advertisement
Print
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Journal of Neuroradiology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Functional MR Imaging Evidence of Altered Functional Activation in Metabolic Syndrome
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Journal of Neuroradiology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Journal of Neuroradiology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Cite this article
K.F. Hoth, M.M. Gonzales, T. Tarumi, S.C. Miles, H. Tanaka, A.P. Haley
Functional MR Imaging Evidence of Altered Functional Activation in Metabolic Syndrome
American Journal of Neuroradiology Mar 2011, 32 (3) 541-547; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A2315

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
0 Responses
Respond to this article
Share
Bookmark this article
Functional MR Imaging Evidence of Altered Functional Activation in Metabolic Syndrome
K.F. Hoth, M.M. Gonzales, T. Tarumi, S.C. Miles, H. Tanaka, A.P. Haley
American Journal of Neuroradiology Mar 2011, 32 (3) 541-547; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A2315
del.icio.us logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
Purchase

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Abbreviations
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusions
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Responses
  • References
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Brain Imaging Changes Associated With Risk Factors for Cardiovascular and Cerebrovascular Disease in Asymptomatic Patients
  • Substituting dietary monounsaturated fat for saturated fat is associated with increased daily physical activity and resting energy expenditure and with changes in mood
  • Impact of Metabolic Syndrome on Cognition and Brain: A Selected Review of the Literature
  • Crossref
  • Google Scholar

This article has not yet been cited by articles in journals that are participating in Crossref Cited-by Linking.

More in this TOC Section

  • Kurtosis and Epileptogenic Tubers: A Pilot Study
  • Glutaric Aciduria Type 1: DK vs. Conventional MRI
  • WM Neuroaxonal Loss in Type 1 Diabetes
Show more Functional

Similar Articles

Advertisement

Indexed Content

  • Current Issue
  • Accepted Manuscripts
  • Article Preview
  • Past Issues
  • Editorials
  • Editor's Choice
  • Fellows' Journal Club
  • Letters to the Editor
  • Video Articles

Cases

  • Case Collection
  • Archive - Case of the Week
  • Archive - Case of the Month
  • Archive - Classic Case

More from AJNR

  • Trainee Corner
  • Imaging Protocols
  • MRI Safety Corner
  • Book Reviews

Multimedia

  • AJNR Podcasts
  • AJNR Scantastics

Resources

  • Turnaround Time
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Submit a Video Article
  • Submit an eLetter to the Editor/Response
  • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
  • Statistical Tips
  • Fast Publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
  • Graphical Abstract Preparation
  • Imaging Protocol Submission
  • Evidence-Based Medicine Level Guide
  • Publishing Checklists
  • Author Policies
  • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
  • News and Updates

About Us

  • About AJNR
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Board Alumni
  • Alerts
  • Permissions
  • Not an AJNR Subscriber? Join Now
  • Advertise with Us
  • Librarian Resources
  • Feedback
  • Terms and Conditions
  • AJNR Editorial Board Alumni

American Society of Neuroradiology

  • Not an ASNR Member? Join Now

© 2025 by the American Society of Neuroradiology All rights, including for text and data mining, AI training, and similar technologies, are reserved.
Print ISSN: 0195-6108 Online ISSN: 1936-959X

Powered by HighWire